|
Humphrey Taylor is the
Chairman of Louis Harris & Associates, Inc.
This article appeared in the
May 4, 1998, edition of The
Polling Report.
Myth and Reality in
Reporting Sampling Error
How the
Media Confuse and
Mislead Readers and Viewers
by Humphrey Taylor
On almost every occasion when we release a new survey, someone in the media will ask,
"What is the margin of error for this survey?" There is only one honest and
accurate answer to this question
-- which I sometimes use to the great confusion of my audience -- and that is, "The
possible margin of error is infinite."
The follow-up from the journalist is often,
"I cant report that. My editor wont let me run a story about surveys
unless I can report the margin of error."
When the media print sentences such as
"the margin of error is plus or minus three percentage points," they strongly
suggest that the results are accurate to within the percentage stated. That is completely
untrue and grossly misleading. The medias intentions are honorable. They want to
warn people about sampling error. But they might be better off assuming -- as most of the readers surely do -- that all
surveys, all opinion polls (and, indeed, all censuses) are estimates, which may be wrong.
This is surely a classic case of a little
knowledge being a dangerous thing. In the real world, "random sampling error" -- or the likelihood that a pure probability sample
would produce replies within a certain band of percentages only because of the sample size
-- is one of the least of our measurement problems. The main problems of survey
measurement, or more accurately mismeasurement, include:
the sample design (for telephone
surveys, how the numbers were selected and how the individuals are selected within the
household);
the non-availability problem (are
people who are available different on the variables we are measuring than the people who
are not available?);
the refusal problem (is the refusal
rate different on the particular variable we are measuring?);
question wording;
question order;
deliberate, or unconscious, lying or
false reporting by respondents;
inappropriate or inadequate
weighting of the data.
In addition, when we are trying to predict
election results, we have several other problems to worry about, including:
differential turnout (a huge factor
in U.S. elections, where predicting who will vote is often harder than measuring how they
are likely to vote);
late-swing (i.e., people changing
their minds after weve surveyed them).
All of these variables have been shown in
various studies to have been sources of error and sometimes of quite substantial error.
Unfortunately, we are unable to quantify the effects of these sources of error on our
results, or to validate our results within any kind of reasonable budget.
For this reason, we (Harris) include a strong
warning in all of the surveys that we publish. Typically, it goes as follows:
In theory, with a sample of this size, one
can say with 95 percent certainty that the results have a statistical precision of plus or
minus __ percentage points of what they would be if the entire adult population had been
polled with complete accuracy. Unfortunately, there are several other possible sources of
error in all polls or surveys that are probably more serious than theoretical calculations
of sampling error. They include refusals to be interviewed (non-response), question
wording and question order, interviewer bias, weighting by demographic control data, and
screening (e.g., for likely voters). It is difficult or impossible to quantify the errors
that may result from these factors.
If journalists are the least bit interested in
all of this -- and alas, most of
them are not -- they may well ask, "If there are so many sources of error in surveys,
why should we bother to read or report any poll results?" To which I normally give
two replies:
1. Well designed,
well conducted surveys work. Their record overall is pretty good. Most social, and
marketing, researchers would be very happy with the average forecasting errors of the
polls (less than 2% on two main candidates in presidential races since 1952). However,
there are enough disasters in the history of election predictions for readers to be
cautious about interpreting the results.
2. (And this is more effective.) I re-word Winston Churchills famous remarks about
democracy and say, "Polls are the worst way of measuring public opinion and public
behavior, or of predicting elections -- except for all of the others."
|
|
|
. |
. |
Polls are the worst way of measuring public opinion and public
behavior, or of predicting electionsexcept for all of the others. |
|
|